Mol. Cells 2016; 39(10): 750-755
Published online October 31, 2016
https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2016.0173
© The Korean Society for Molecular and Cellular Biology
Correspondence to : *Correspondence: haejin31@gmail.com (HJK); zebrakim@cnu.ac.kr (CHK)
Although innate color preference of motile organisms may provide clues to behavioral biases, it has remained a longstanding question. In this study, we investigated innate color preference of zebrafish larvae. A cross maze with different color sleeves around each arm was used for the color preference test (R; red, G; green, B; blue, Y; yellow). The findings showed that 5 dpf zebrafish larvae preferred blue over other colors (B > R > G > Y). To study innate color recognition further,
Keywords autism, behavioral analysis, color blindness, color preference, zebrafish
Color vision is one of the most important modalities in the recognition of biologically important stimulation, and thus it plays a critical role in visual perception. Depending on the number of cone types, animals including humans can perceive colors based on the distribution of the wavelengths of light. Humans and some primates have trichromatic vision, but most non-primate mammals are dichromatic; many lower vertebrates, such as birds and fish, are tetrachromatic. Phylogenetic analysis of opsin gene sequences suggests that cones were the ancestral pigments with rods evolving afterward. It is yet to be thoroughly investigated, but the nocturnal bottleneck hypothesis explains this counterintuitive fact (Bowmaker, 2008; Gerkema et al., 2013; Jacobs, 2009; Robertson et al., 2004) (Fig. 1). Many groups point to the possibility that, although most mammals lack one or more opsin when compared to fish and primates, the rest of the machinery and strategies used in color processing were not lost through evolution (Joselevitch and Kamermans, 2009).
Innate color preference or aversion aids in the recognition of one’s environment and can provide information on how to appropriately respond to environmental stimuli. However, limited research has been conducted in this area. In humans, gender-stereotyped color preference was reported in a study involving young female subjects who demonstrated increasing attraction to pink over blue, whereas male counterparts displayed growing avoidance of pink (LoBue and DeLoache, 2011). Colors within the visible spectrum are determined by the wavelength of light, thus color recognition relies on the cones of the retina. Zebrafish (
Zebrafish eyes detect light by 3.5 dpf, and zebrafish larva displays mobility starting from 5 dpf. Based on this information, an innate color preference test can be designed to assay as soon as zebrafish larvae begin to swim. This arrangement ensures protection from all color stimuli until experiments begin, which negates postnatal color adaptation. One previous color preference study showed that adult zebrafish have a strong aversion towards blue compared to red, green, and yellow colors. Avdesh et al. (2012) suggested that zebrafish (25 weeks) have natural color preferences without any rewarded stimuli. However, innate color preference studies of laboratory-available fishes, such as zebrafish, guppies, and medaka, have not yet received ample attention in the scientific literature.
Oculocutaneous albinism (OCA) is a group of inherited disorders characterized by defective melanin biosynthesis which leads to a reduction or a complete lack of melanin pigment in the hair, skin, and eyes. Clinical manifestations of OCA include reduced pigmentation of the RPE and impaired color vision. Tyrosinase is the rate-limiting enzyme involved in melanin biosynthesis. Abnormal tyrosinase caused by errors occurred during post-translational modification or trafficking into melanosomes can decrease pigmentation in skin and RPE (Grønskov et al., 2007). Previous studies demonstrated that the CRISPR/Cas9 system can be used to efficiently induce a site-specific modification in zebrafish (Singh and N?sslein-Volhard, 2015). CRISPR/Cas9 system makes highly specific cuts in DNA molecules and carries out insertions or deletions at the sequence sites.
Zebrafish display a broad spectrum of behaviors such as learning, cognition, aggression, anxiety and social interaction (Fadool and Dowling, 2008; Miller et al., 2015; Orefice et al., 2016; Richendrfer et al., 2012). With the innate color preference test, we have a simple way to measure quantitative effects of small molecules and observe behavioral changes due to blindness or neurodevelopmental disorders at a very early larval stage. To understand the molecular mechanisms underlying animal behaviors, we established an
All experiments using zebrafish were performed in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Chungnam National University (CNU-00393).
Wild-type zebrafish were purchased from local pet stores (Korea) and used as breeders. 50~100 fertilized eggs were raised in 50 ml egg water (sea salt 60 ug/ml) in a transparent 90 mm culture dish (10091, SPL, Korea) at 28.5°C with a 14 h light on/10hr light off cycle under approximately 3000 Lux fluorescent light. After hatching, larvae swam freely without any disturbance until 7 dpf, then reared as previously described (May et al., 2015). A combination of granule (Hatchfry Encapsulon grade 0, Argent Lab, USA) and brine shrimp was fed from 8 dpf, and then switched to brine shrimp from 16 dpf.
One-cell stage zebrafish embryos were injected with 350 ng/μl Cas9 mRNA and 100 ng/μl sgRNA. tyr sgRNA and Cas9 mRNA injected embryos shows 70?80% mutation at tyr loci. PCR primers for the genotyping of the zebrafish
Color preference test was performed either in a cross (Figs. 2 and 6B) or bidirectional color maze (Figs. 3 and 6A). The maze was built using 3mm clear acrylic sheets. The cross maze had four arms, and the bidirectional color maze had two arms in a transparent acryl plate. The dimension of each arm was 10 (W) × 35 (L) × 15 (H) mm. The maze was left uncovered on top. Different color sleeves could be placed on the outside of each arm to provide different color cues (N; no color sleeve, R; red, G; green, B; blue, Y; yellow, M; magenta, C; cyan). The maze was implemented to have 20 zebrafish larvae (from 4 dpf to 30 dpf) freely swimming in all direction. With color sleeves, zebrafish larvae (n = 40) swam freely during the 30 min experiment. The location of each larva was indicated with a red dot every 2 min for a total duration of 30 min of video recording (Fig. 2), and the number of larvae in each arm was manually counted. To eliminate any unexpected bias, color sleeves were moved to opposite arms after each experiment.
The transmitted wavelength spectrum of all color sleeves and stand light were measured using a spectrometer (SV2100-VIS, K-Mac, Korea). In the penetration wavelength spectrum measurement, a commercial stand light with a typical cool white fluorescent lamp (FPL27EX-D, Jianxi South Lightings, China) was used as a light source. Light intensity was measured with a lux meter (TES-1335, TES, Taiwan). The spectrum of the lamp had main peaks including red (607 nm), green (546 nm) and blue (435 nm) wavelengths. Yellow is comprised of red and green. Magenta is comprised of blue and red. Cyan is comprised of blue and green.
All maze experiments were recorded using a high-resolution digital camera (HDR-CX130, Sony, Japan). The video file was first stored on the camera’s memory card and subsequently transferred to the computer’s hard drive. The full-HD video file that was produced in the MTS format from the digital camera was approximately 2 gigabyte/file. The number of zebrafish larvae (n = 40) in each color zone was counted manually every 2 minutes for 30 min (Fig. 2). To compare the Wild-type and
For paraffin embedding, embryos were dehydrated with ethanol for 5 min and washed for 10 min in isopropanol. They were then cleared three times for 10 min in Xylene. Embryos were transferred to xylene:paraffin (1:1) solution at 60°C for 20 min and then embedded three times for 20 min with paraffin in a stainless steel mold. Embryos were held in position in the pre-warmed plastic embedding cassette. After trimming, sections (8 μm) were cut by a microtome and deparaffinized with Xylene. The preparations were mounted with Canada balsam and examined by LEICA DM5000B microscope.
All data and graphs were expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) and analyzed by an independent t-test using the statistics program (SPSS, IBM, USA). All figures were adjusted for size using Photoshop.
A cross maze with four arms was developed as a four color preference test (Fig. 2). To test RGBY color preference, the cross maze was fitted with color sleeves (R, G, B, and Y) placed one on each arm, and 5 dpf zebrafish larvae (n = 40) were introduced and allowed to swim freely. The number of larvae in each arm was counted every 2 min for 30 min total (Figs. 2A and 2B). In the control experiment without color sleeves, the larvae were uniformly distributed (Fig. 2C); however, experimental data showed that more larvae preferred to stay in B (19.57 ± 1.76) and R zones (13.70 ± 1.60) than in G and Y. Thus, innate color preference of zebrafish larvae was determined as B > R >> G > Y. This color test showed that zebrafish larvae exhibited both color discrimination and distinct color preference. To eliminate group effect, a single larva was tested in a bidirectional color maze and the same color preference was again observed (data not shown). Also, pre-exposure to color stimulus (B, Y) did not alter innate color preference of zebrafish larvae (data not shown).
A bidirectional maze was developed to compare color preference between two colors chosen from R, G, B, Y, C or M. This test yielded a similar result to that of the color cross maze - larvae preferred B and R over G and Y, and the order of RGBY preference was B > R >> G > Y (Fig. 3).
Between the primary (R/G/B) and secondary (Y) colors, larvae showed a greater preference for B and R over Y (Figs. 3D and 3E). Larva also had a tendency to stay more in the G zone (12.14 ± 0.194) than in the Y zone (G > Y) (7.86 ± 0.194) (P < 0.001). In the case of magenta, B (12.00 ± 0.148) was preferred over M (8.00 ± 0.148), and M (12.71 ± 0.140) was preferred over R (7.29 ± 1.536), suggesting a color preference of B > M > R. In the case of cyan, B (13.13 ± 0.133) was preferred over C (6.87 ± 0.133), and C (16.24 ± 0.152) was preferred over G (3.76 ± 0.152), and suggested an order of B > C > G. No distinct preference was observed between R and C. Overall, the order of RGBYMC preference was B > M > R, C >> G > Y.
OCA is a heterogeneous autosomal recessive genetic disorder of melanin synthesis. OCA results in reduced or absent pigmentation in the hair, skin, and eyes. In the eye, a pigmented cell layer called RPE shields the neurosensory retina from excess incoming light. Reduced pigmentation of RPE and color vision impairment are known symptoms of OCA patients. Among many types of OCA, type 1 OCA (OCA1) occurs as a result of
The color preference test of
The data showed WT larvae preferred B over Y in the bidirectional maze and spent more than 20 min in the B zone during the 30-min experiment. On the other hand,
In pharmaceutical studies, zebrafish larvae have recently been utilized in high-throughput applications to determine the quantitative effects of small molecules on rest, wake, motor behavior and cognition (Miller et al., 2015; Richendrfer et al., 2012). This study examined innate color preference using zebrafish larvae. Such investigations involving innate color recognition and response should be designed to avoid postnatal adaptation and address the subtle responses to monochromatic light in daytime (Sugimoto et al., 2005).
During the course of our research, Peeters et al. (2016) also reported on the color preference of zebrafish. The findings showed that normal larvae preferred blue and avoided yellow. When we tested primary and secondary colors, zebrafish larvae preferred B over M, and M over R. The same tendency was observed in B, C, and G - B was preferred over C and C was preferred over G; however Y was avoided in all cases. This phenomenon was consistent throughout the experiment despite maze shape, suggesting that zebrafish larvae are instinctively attracted to blue and avoided yellow which is the complement of blue. Environmental factors such as light intensity or pre-exposure to colors did not affect innate color preference, and the number of tested larvae (1~40 larvae) was not shown to affect the result, negating group effect (Supplementary Fig. 1).
To further test innate color preference of zebrafish larvae, a color blindness model (OCA-like zebrafish) was generated using the CRISPR/Cas9 system.
In this study, a simple color preference test in zebrafish was established, and using CRISPR/Cas9, a zebrafish OCA model which has hypopigmented RPE resulting in color blindness was generated. Color sensation of zebrafish is very unique, and especially B over Y color recognition is distinctive enough to be applied to various genetic and pharmaceutical studies (Supplementary Fig. 2). This color preference test will allow high throughput screening studies to determine the quantitative effects of small molecules on color cognition and motor behavior.
Mol. Cells 2016; 39(10): 750-755
Published online October 31, 2016 https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2016.0173
Copyright © The Korean Society for Molecular and Cellular Biology.
Jong-Su Park1,4, Jae-Ho Ryu1,4, Tae-Ik Choi1, Young-Ki Bae2, Suman Lee3, Hae Jin Kang1,*, and Cheol-Hee Kim1,*
1Department of Biology, Chungnam National University, Daejeon 34134, Korea, 2Comparative Biomedicine Research Branch, National Cancer Center, Goyang 10408, Korea, 3Division of Structural and Functional Genomics, Center for Genome Science, National Research Institute of Health, Osong 28159, Korea
Correspondence to:*Correspondence: haejin31@gmail.com (HJK); zebrakim@cnu.ac.kr (CHK)
Although innate color preference of motile organisms may provide clues to behavioral biases, it has remained a longstanding question. In this study, we investigated innate color preference of zebrafish larvae. A cross maze with different color sleeves around each arm was used for the color preference test (R; red, G; green, B; blue, Y; yellow). The findings showed that 5 dpf zebrafish larvae preferred blue over other colors (B > R > G > Y). To study innate color recognition further,
Keywords: autism, behavioral analysis, color blindness, color preference, zebrafish
Color vision is one of the most important modalities in the recognition of biologically important stimulation, and thus it plays a critical role in visual perception. Depending on the number of cone types, animals including humans can perceive colors based on the distribution of the wavelengths of light. Humans and some primates have trichromatic vision, but most non-primate mammals are dichromatic; many lower vertebrates, such as birds and fish, are tetrachromatic. Phylogenetic analysis of opsin gene sequences suggests that cones were the ancestral pigments with rods evolving afterward. It is yet to be thoroughly investigated, but the nocturnal bottleneck hypothesis explains this counterintuitive fact (Bowmaker, 2008; Gerkema et al., 2013; Jacobs, 2009; Robertson et al., 2004) (Fig. 1). Many groups point to the possibility that, although most mammals lack one or more opsin when compared to fish and primates, the rest of the machinery and strategies used in color processing were not lost through evolution (Joselevitch and Kamermans, 2009).
Innate color preference or aversion aids in the recognition of one’s environment and can provide information on how to appropriately respond to environmental stimuli. However, limited research has been conducted in this area. In humans, gender-stereotyped color preference was reported in a study involving young female subjects who demonstrated increasing attraction to pink over blue, whereas male counterparts displayed growing avoidance of pink (LoBue and DeLoache, 2011). Colors within the visible spectrum are determined by the wavelength of light, thus color recognition relies on the cones of the retina. Zebrafish (
Zebrafish eyes detect light by 3.5 dpf, and zebrafish larva displays mobility starting from 5 dpf. Based on this information, an innate color preference test can be designed to assay as soon as zebrafish larvae begin to swim. This arrangement ensures protection from all color stimuli until experiments begin, which negates postnatal color adaptation. One previous color preference study showed that adult zebrafish have a strong aversion towards blue compared to red, green, and yellow colors. Avdesh et al. (2012) suggested that zebrafish (25 weeks) have natural color preferences without any rewarded stimuli. However, innate color preference studies of laboratory-available fishes, such as zebrafish, guppies, and medaka, have not yet received ample attention in the scientific literature.
Oculocutaneous albinism (OCA) is a group of inherited disorders characterized by defective melanin biosynthesis which leads to a reduction or a complete lack of melanin pigment in the hair, skin, and eyes. Clinical manifestations of OCA include reduced pigmentation of the RPE and impaired color vision. Tyrosinase is the rate-limiting enzyme involved in melanin biosynthesis. Abnormal tyrosinase caused by errors occurred during post-translational modification or trafficking into melanosomes can decrease pigmentation in skin and RPE (Grønskov et al., 2007). Previous studies demonstrated that the CRISPR/Cas9 system can be used to efficiently induce a site-specific modification in zebrafish (Singh and N?sslein-Volhard, 2015). CRISPR/Cas9 system makes highly specific cuts in DNA molecules and carries out insertions or deletions at the sequence sites.
Zebrafish display a broad spectrum of behaviors such as learning, cognition, aggression, anxiety and social interaction (Fadool and Dowling, 2008; Miller et al., 2015; Orefice et al., 2016; Richendrfer et al., 2012). With the innate color preference test, we have a simple way to measure quantitative effects of small molecules and observe behavioral changes due to blindness or neurodevelopmental disorders at a very early larval stage. To understand the molecular mechanisms underlying animal behaviors, we established an
All experiments using zebrafish were performed in accordance with the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Chungnam National University (CNU-00393).
Wild-type zebrafish were purchased from local pet stores (Korea) and used as breeders. 50~100 fertilized eggs were raised in 50 ml egg water (sea salt 60 ug/ml) in a transparent 90 mm culture dish (10091, SPL, Korea) at 28.5°C with a 14 h light on/10hr light off cycle under approximately 3000 Lux fluorescent light. After hatching, larvae swam freely without any disturbance until 7 dpf, then reared as previously described (May et al., 2015). A combination of granule (Hatchfry Encapsulon grade 0, Argent Lab, USA) and brine shrimp was fed from 8 dpf, and then switched to brine shrimp from 16 dpf.
One-cell stage zebrafish embryos were injected with 350 ng/μl Cas9 mRNA and 100 ng/μl sgRNA. tyr sgRNA and Cas9 mRNA injected embryos shows 70?80% mutation at tyr loci. PCR primers for the genotyping of the zebrafish
Color preference test was performed either in a cross (Figs. 2 and 6B) or bidirectional color maze (Figs. 3 and 6A). The maze was built using 3mm clear acrylic sheets. The cross maze had four arms, and the bidirectional color maze had two arms in a transparent acryl plate. The dimension of each arm was 10 (W) × 35 (L) × 15 (H) mm. The maze was left uncovered on top. Different color sleeves could be placed on the outside of each arm to provide different color cues (N; no color sleeve, R; red, G; green, B; blue, Y; yellow, M; magenta, C; cyan). The maze was implemented to have 20 zebrafish larvae (from 4 dpf to 30 dpf) freely swimming in all direction. With color sleeves, zebrafish larvae (n = 40) swam freely during the 30 min experiment. The location of each larva was indicated with a red dot every 2 min for a total duration of 30 min of video recording (Fig. 2), and the number of larvae in each arm was manually counted. To eliminate any unexpected bias, color sleeves were moved to opposite arms after each experiment.
The transmitted wavelength spectrum of all color sleeves and stand light were measured using a spectrometer (SV2100-VIS, K-Mac, Korea). In the penetration wavelength spectrum measurement, a commercial stand light with a typical cool white fluorescent lamp (FPL27EX-D, Jianxi South Lightings, China) was used as a light source. Light intensity was measured with a lux meter (TES-1335, TES, Taiwan). The spectrum of the lamp had main peaks including red (607 nm), green (546 nm) and blue (435 nm) wavelengths. Yellow is comprised of red and green. Magenta is comprised of blue and red. Cyan is comprised of blue and green.
All maze experiments were recorded using a high-resolution digital camera (HDR-CX130, Sony, Japan). The video file was first stored on the camera’s memory card and subsequently transferred to the computer’s hard drive. The full-HD video file that was produced in the MTS format from the digital camera was approximately 2 gigabyte/file. The number of zebrafish larvae (n = 40) in each color zone was counted manually every 2 minutes for 30 min (Fig. 2). To compare the Wild-type and
For paraffin embedding, embryos were dehydrated with ethanol for 5 min and washed for 10 min in isopropanol. They were then cleared three times for 10 min in Xylene. Embryos were transferred to xylene:paraffin (1:1) solution at 60°C for 20 min and then embedded three times for 20 min with paraffin in a stainless steel mold. Embryos were held in position in the pre-warmed plastic embedding cassette. After trimming, sections (8 μm) were cut by a microtome and deparaffinized with Xylene. The preparations were mounted with Canada balsam and examined by LEICA DM5000B microscope.
All data and graphs were expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) and analyzed by an independent t-test using the statistics program (SPSS, IBM, USA). All figures were adjusted for size using Photoshop.
A cross maze with four arms was developed as a four color preference test (Fig. 2). To test RGBY color preference, the cross maze was fitted with color sleeves (R, G, B, and Y) placed one on each arm, and 5 dpf zebrafish larvae (n = 40) were introduced and allowed to swim freely. The number of larvae in each arm was counted every 2 min for 30 min total (Figs. 2A and 2B). In the control experiment without color sleeves, the larvae were uniformly distributed (Fig. 2C); however, experimental data showed that more larvae preferred to stay in B (19.57 ± 1.76) and R zones (13.70 ± 1.60) than in G and Y. Thus, innate color preference of zebrafish larvae was determined as B > R >> G > Y. This color test showed that zebrafish larvae exhibited both color discrimination and distinct color preference. To eliminate group effect, a single larva was tested in a bidirectional color maze and the same color preference was again observed (data not shown). Also, pre-exposure to color stimulus (B, Y) did not alter innate color preference of zebrafish larvae (data not shown).
A bidirectional maze was developed to compare color preference between two colors chosen from R, G, B, Y, C or M. This test yielded a similar result to that of the color cross maze - larvae preferred B and R over G and Y, and the order of RGBY preference was B > R >> G > Y (Fig. 3).
Between the primary (R/G/B) and secondary (Y) colors, larvae showed a greater preference for B and R over Y (Figs. 3D and 3E). Larva also had a tendency to stay more in the G zone (12.14 ± 0.194) than in the Y zone (G > Y) (7.86 ± 0.194) (P < 0.001). In the case of magenta, B (12.00 ± 0.148) was preferred over M (8.00 ± 0.148), and M (12.71 ± 0.140) was preferred over R (7.29 ± 1.536), suggesting a color preference of B > M > R. In the case of cyan, B (13.13 ± 0.133) was preferred over C (6.87 ± 0.133), and C (16.24 ± 0.152) was preferred over G (3.76 ± 0.152), and suggested an order of B > C > G. No distinct preference was observed between R and C. Overall, the order of RGBYMC preference was B > M > R, C >> G > Y.
OCA is a heterogeneous autosomal recessive genetic disorder of melanin synthesis. OCA results in reduced or absent pigmentation in the hair, skin, and eyes. In the eye, a pigmented cell layer called RPE shields the neurosensory retina from excess incoming light. Reduced pigmentation of RPE and color vision impairment are known symptoms of OCA patients. Among many types of OCA, type 1 OCA (OCA1) occurs as a result of
The color preference test of
The data showed WT larvae preferred B over Y in the bidirectional maze and spent more than 20 min in the B zone during the 30-min experiment. On the other hand,
In pharmaceutical studies, zebrafish larvae have recently been utilized in high-throughput applications to determine the quantitative effects of small molecules on rest, wake, motor behavior and cognition (Miller et al., 2015; Richendrfer et al., 2012). This study examined innate color preference using zebrafish larvae. Such investigations involving innate color recognition and response should be designed to avoid postnatal adaptation and address the subtle responses to monochromatic light in daytime (Sugimoto et al., 2005).
During the course of our research, Peeters et al. (2016) also reported on the color preference of zebrafish. The findings showed that normal larvae preferred blue and avoided yellow. When we tested primary and secondary colors, zebrafish larvae preferred B over M, and M over R. The same tendency was observed in B, C, and G - B was preferred over C and C was preferred over G; however Y was avoided in all cases. This phenomenon was consistent throughout the experiment despite maze shape, suggesting that zebrafish larvae are instinctively attracted to blue and avoided yellow which is the complement of blue. Environmental factors such as light intensity or pre-exposure to colors did not affect innate color preference, and the number of tested larvae (1~40 larvae) was not shown to affect the result, negating group effect (Supplementary Fig. 1).
To further test innate color preference of zebrafish larvae, a color blindness model (OCA-like zebrafish) was generated using the CRISPR/Cas9 system.
In this study, a simple color preference test in zebrafish was established, and using CRISPR/Cas9, a zebrafish OCA model which has hypopigmented RPE resulting in color blindness was generated. Color sensation of zebrafish is very unique, and especially B over Y color recognition is distinctive enough to be applied to various genetic and pharmaceutical studies (Supplementary Fig. 2). This color preference test will allow high throughput screening studies to determine the quantitative effects of small molecules on color cognition and motor behavior.
Kyeong-Won Yoo, Maivannan Thiruvarangan, Yun-Mi Jeong, Mi-Sun Lee, Sateesh Maddirevula, Myungchull Rhee, Young-Ki Bae, Hyung-Goo Kim, and Cheol-Hee Kim
Mol. Cells 2017; 40(4): 271-279 https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2017.2308Sang Mee Park, Hae Ryoun Park, and Ji Hye Lee
Mol. Cells 2017; 40(2): 151-161 https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2017.2307Chan-Mi Park, Hye-Min Kim, Dong Hyun Kim, Ho-Jin Han, Haneul Noh, Jae-Hyuk Jang, Soo-Hyun Park, Han-Jung Chae, Soo-Wan Chae, Eun Kyoung Ryu, Sangku Lee, Kangdong Liu, Haidan Liu, Jong-Seog Ahn, Young Ock Kim, Bo-Yeon Kim, and Nak-Kyun Soung
Mol. Cells 2016; 39(12): 855-861 https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2016.0111