Mol. Cells 2016; 39(4): 310-315
Published online February 25, 2016
https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2016.2250
© The Korean Society for Molecular and Cellular Biology
Correspondence to : *Correspondence: ylee@kookmin.ac.kr
Coumarin is a phenolic compound that mainly affects the liver due to its metabolization into a toxic compound. The deterrent and ovicidal activities of coumarin in insect models such as
Keywords bitter, chemosensation, feeding, oviposition, taste
Every living being, from microbes to mammals, requires food for survival. Food consists of a number of complex compounds; some are beneficial, some are allergenic, while others may be harmful. The latter are commonly termed toxic compounds. There are numerous naturally occurring toxic metabolites in food. Phenol is one of these metabolites. A study found that phenol shows either carcinogenic or toxic effects in animal models (Smith et al., 1989). In fact, a phenolic metabolite, coumarin, is banned in food products (Dolan et al., 2010; Singleton, 1981). These toxic metabolites are normally produced by plants in order to protect themselves from insects and pests.
The taste sensory system normally functions in discriminating nutritious foods from non-nutritious ones. One part of the taste system controls acceptance behavior, while the other regulates avoidance behavior. The taste receptor in
Previous studies of insect taste receptors have been performed on the sugar receptor. A common clade in the phylogenetic tree of 68
The function of the GR receptor in detecting bitter compounds serves as an advantage to the fly, in terms of specifying the site for oviposition behavior (Joseph and Herberlein, 2012; Yang et al., 2008). In addition, the olfactory receptor (OR) also plays a role in substrate selection. The main aim of female flies when choosing substrate choice for oviposition, is to safeguard their progeny from parasites and the deleterious effects of toxic bitter compounds, as well as to provide a source of nourishment of their progeny. Similarly, the females choose to lay eggs in fermenting substrates containing ethanol and citrus fruits so as to protect their eggs and larvae from endoparasitoid wasps. The latter behavior is controlled by olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) expressing Or19a+ (Dweck et al., 2013; Kacsoh et al., 2013).
Here, we show that coumarin has a toxic effect when fed in a dosage-dependent manner. Anti-feeding behavior to avoid coumarin-laced food is regulated by aversive GRNs, but not ORNs. Furthermore, we identified the possible coumarin receptor using six previously verified mutants, by binary food choice assay, assessment of proboscis extension response assay and electrophysiology measurement. Finally, we found that only
Sucrose, coumarin, and sulforhodamine B were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Co. Brilliant blue FCF was ordered from Wako Pure Chemical Industry Ltd.
We performed binary food choice assays as described previously (Meunier et al., 2003; Moon et al., 2006). Firstly, 50?70 flies that were 3?6 days old were starved for 18 h in a humidified chamber. We prepared two different food substrates with 1% agarose: one containing 1 mM sucrose, and the other containing 5 mM sucrose with different concentration of coumarin. These food substrates were mixed with either one of two food coloring dyes, i.e. one was mixed with blue dye (brilliant blue FCF, 0.125 mg/ml) while the other was mixed with red dye (sulforhodamine B, 0.2 mg/ml). We distributed the mixture of two food sources in a 72-well microtiter dish, in alternative fashion, and then we introduced the starved flies into the dish. The flies in the microtiter dish were kept in a dark, humidified chamber, and allowed to feed for 90 min at room temperature. To sacrifice the flies, we kept them at ?20°C and then analyzed the color of their abdomens by microscopy. Blue (NB), red (NR), or purple (NP) flies were counted. The preference index (P.I.) was calculated according to the following equation: (NB+0.5NP)/(NR+NB+NP) or (NR+0.5NP)/(NR+NB+NP), depending on the dye/tastant combinations. P.I.s = 1.0 and 0 indicated complete preferences for either 1 mM or 5 mM sucrose, with or without coumarin, respectively. A P.I. = 0.5 indicated no bias between the two food choices.
PER assay was performed as previously described (Lee et al., 2015), with slight modification. The concentration of sucrose used for the initial stimuli was 2%, and then 10 mM coumarin was applied with 2% sucrose. The flies that did not respond to sucrose as a positive stimulant were discarded. Kim-wipe paper wicks were used as media to provide flies with tastant stimuli. Wet wicks were gently brought in contact with the proboscis. Prolonged contact with the stimulating agent may give a negative result. Water, which acts as a negative stimulant, was given to the flies as described above. Flies showing proboscis extension in response to this negative stimulant were discarded. Next, the test solution, i.e. 10 mM coumarin in 2% sucrose stimuli, was given, and positive PER was calculated. The test was performed for 10 flies at a time, and the positive PER for each fly was calculated as 10% proboscis extension. The test was repeated four times for each fly strain, i.e. mutant, control, and rescue fly strains.
We performed tip recordings as previously described (Moon et al., 2006). We immobilized freshly enclosed flies by keeping them on ice and then inserted reference glass electrodes filled with Ringer’s solution into the thorax of the flies, extending the electrode towards their proboscis. We stimulated the sensilla with tastants dissolved in buffer solution in recording pipettes (10?20 μm tip diameter). We used 1 mM KCl or 30 mM tricholine citrate as the electrolyte for recording. The recording electrode was connected to a preamplifier (TastePROBE, Syntech, Hilversum, The Netherlands), and the signals were collected and amplified 10x, using a signal connection interface box (Syntech) in conjunction with a 100?3000 Hz band-pass filter. Recordings of action potentials were acquired using a 12-kHz sampling rate, and analyzed using Autospike 3.1 software (Syntech). First, we performed recordings on S6 sensilla with different concentrations of coumarin i.e. 0 mM, 0.1 mM and 1 mM for control flies. Finally, we performed recordings with the mutant and rescue flies from S5, S6 and S9 sensilla with 1 mM coumarin.
We developed our own protocol for egg laying (Poudel et al., 2015). Fifteen male and 15 female flies, all 2?3 days-old, were kept in a fresh food source and incubated at 25°C for 2 days. The experiment was divided into two parts: 6 h adaptation (starvation), and 18 h egg laying period, which completes 24 h circadian rhythm. For starvation, we kept the flies on 1% agarose on the egg laying apparatus, which was a 5 cm diameter petri dish divided by 4 mm spacer. We started the experiment exactly at 12 PM for starvation, and then at 6 PM we transferred the flies to the petri dish containing 1% agarose, with either 1 mM sucrose, or 5 mM sucrose plus indicated concentrations for coumarin. The flies were then allowed to lay eggs for 18 h. The flies were kept in a dark and humidified chamber for both starvation and oviposition. The numbers of eggs laid over 18 h were counted. The oviposition index was calculated, as previously described (Yang et al., 2008).
We performed survival assays with the control flies. We prepared three different combinations of food sources: one with 1% sucrose, and the other two with 1% sucrose plus 1 mM and 10 mM coumarin. We placed 10 male and 10 female flies, 3?4 days-old, on each of these food sources. The flies were observed every 12 h, and then transferred to new vials containing the same food source. The assay was performed for 72 h, by which time all the flies feeding on coumarin containing media were dead. The test was repeated 10 times.
All error bars represent SEMs. Single factor ANOVA with Scheffe’s analysis was used as a
Coumarin, a fragrant organic chemical compound, is considered a phenylpropanoid (Fig. 1A). It is produced by plants as a defense mechanism to repel predators. Coumarin is moderately toxic to the liver and kidneys in mammals (Lake, 1999); therefore, we decided to test its toxicity on insect models. The toxicity of coumarin was investigated by performing survival assay with control flies. Flies maintained in 1% sucrose lived for more than 72 h (Fig. 1B). However, 1 mM coumarin was sufficient to kill 50% of the flies in 45 h (LT50). This was reduced to 30 h (LT50) for 10 mM coumarin (Fig. 1B). All coumarin-feeding flies were dead before 72 h. This indicates that coumarin is also toxic to insects.
Coumarin is known to have a pleasant smell to humans. However, it also has a somewhat bitter-tasting anti-feedant effect. To determine whether the repellent behavior is actually mediated by its bitterness or by its odor, we expressed a proapoptosis gene (
To find the
The fly has multiple contact-chemosensory organs including a proboscis, legs, wings and genitalia. The main contact-chemosensory organs are the proboscis and legs. To further verify the roles of GRs in the labellum, we performed the proboscis extension response (PER) assay for coumarin with defective
Next, we performed tip recordings to elicit coumarin-induced action potentials from S5, S6 and S9 sensilla, which are known to be highly activated by coumarin (Weiss et al., 2011). Consistent with the behavioral defects of
These data support the hypothesis that the activity of GRs in the labellum is necessary to discriminate between nutritious food sources and toxic foods.
Recent study provides the evidence that three bitter-sensing GRs are enough to recapitulate L-canavanine receptor (GR33a, GR66a, and GR98b) in sweet neurons (Shim et al., 2015). To recapitulate a coumarin receptor in sweet neurons ectopically, we generated the flies expressing all the three
The site selection for the deposition of the egg in fruit fly is crucial for the safety of the developing larvae. The studies done till date support the idea that flies sense media using smell and taste for the oviposition in Drosophila (Dweck et al., 2013; Joseph and Herberlein, 2012; Yang et al., 2008).
Coumarin is an ovicidal compound (Nakajima and Kawazu, 1980). So our assumption was that female flies should avoid a coumarin-containing substrate for egg laying. We carried out egg laying assay with the same condition as binary food choice assay. 0.3 mM coumarin was not enough for the female flies to avoid egg laying as binary food choice assay (Figs. 1C, 4A, and 4B). Interestingly, only
The olfactory receptor is composed of Orco and specific OR, which resides in specific ORNs. However, GRs are distributed on overlapped sensilla. For example,
Mol. Cells 2016; 39(4): 310-315
Published online April 30, 2016 https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2016.2250
Copyright © The Korean Society for Molecular and Cellular Biology.
Seeta Poudel, and Youngseok Lee*
Department of Bio and Fermentation Convergence Technology, BK21 PLUS Project, Kookmin University, Seoul 136-702, Korea
Correspondence to:*Correspondence: ylee@kookmin.ac.kr
Coumarin is a phenolic compound that mainly affects the liver due to its metabolization into a toxic compound. The deterrent and ovicidal activities of coumarin in insect models such as
Keywords: bitter, chemosensation, feeding, oviposition, taste
Every living being, from microbes to mammals, requires food for survival. Food consists of a number of complex compounds; some are beneficial, some are allergenic, while others may be harmful. The latter are commonly termed toxic compounds. There are numerous naturally occurring toxic metabolites in food. Phenol is one of these metabolites. A study found that phenol shows either carcinogenic or toxic effects in animal models (Smith et al., 1989). In fact, a phenolic metabolite, coumarin, is banned in food products (Dolan et al., 2010; Singleton, 1981). These toxic metabolites are normally produced by plants in order to protect themselves from insects and pests.
The taste sensory system normally functions in discriminating nutritious foods from non-nutritious ones. One part of the taste system controls acceptance behavior, while the other regulates avoidance behavior. The taste receptor in
Previous studies of insect taste receptors have been performed on the sugar receptor. A common clade in the phylogenetic tree of 68
The function of the GR receptor in detecting bitter compounds serves as an advantage to the fly, in terms of specifying the site for oviposition behavior (Joseph and Herberlein, 2012; Yang et al., 2008). In addition, the olfactory receptor (OR) also plays a role in substrate selection. The main aim of female flies when choosing substrate choice for oviposition, is to safeguard their progeny from parasites and the deleterious effects of toxic bitter compounds, as well as to provide a source of nourishment of their progeny. Similarly, the females choose to lay eggs in fermenting substrates containing ethanol and citrus fruits so as to protect their eggs and larvae from endoparasitoid wasps. The latter behavior is controlled by olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) expressing Or19a+ (Dweck et al., 2013; Kacsoh et al., 2013).
Here, we show that coumarin has a toxic effect when fed in a dosage-dependent manner. Anti-feeding behavior to avoid coumarin-laced food is regulated by aversive GRNs, but not ORNs. Furthermore, we identified the possible coumarin receptor using six previously verified mutants, by binary food choice assay, assessment of proboscis extension response assay and electrophysiology measurement. Finally, we found that only
Sucrose, coumarin, and sulforhodamine B were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Co. Brilliant blue FCF was ordered from Wako Pure Chemical Industry Ltd.
We performed binary food choice assays as described previously (Meunier et al., 2003; Moon et al., 2006). Firstly, 50?70 flies that were 3?6 days old were starved for 18 h in a humidified chamber. We prepared two different food substrates with 1% agarose: one containing 1 mM sucrose, and the other containing 5 mM sucrose with different concentration of coumarin. These food substrates were mixed with either one of two food coloring dyes, i.e. one was mixed with blue dye (brilliant blue FCF, 0.125 mg/ml) while the other was mixed with red dye (sulforhodamine B, 0.2 mg/ml). We distributed the mixture of two food sources in a 72-well microtiter dish, in alternative fashion, and then we introduced the starved flies into the dish. The flies in the microtiter dish were kept in a dark, humidified chamber, and allowed to feed for 90 min at room temperature. To sacrifice the flies, we kept them at ?20°C and then analyzed the color of their abdomens by microscopy. Blue (NB), red (NR), or purple (NP) flies were counted. The preference index (P.I.) was calculated according to the following equation: (NB+0.5NP)/(NR+NB+NP) or (NR+0.5NP)/(NR+NB+NP), depending on the dye/tastant combinations. P.I.s = 1.0 and 0 indicated complete preferences for either 1 mM or 5 mM sucrose, with or without coumarin, respectively. A P.I. = 0.5 indicated no bias between the two food choices.
PER assay was performed as previously described (Lee et al., 2015), with slight modification. The concentration of sucrose used for the initial stimuli was 2%, and then 10 mM coumarin was applied with 2% sucrose. The flies that did not respond to sucrose as a positive stimulant were discarded. Kim-wipe paper wicks were used as media to provide flies with tastant stimuli. Wet wicks were gently brought in contact with the proboscis. Prolonged contact with the stimulating agent may give a negative result. Water, which acts as a negative stimulant, was given to the flies as described above. Flies showing proboscis extension in response to this negative stimulant were discarded. Next, the test solution, i.e. 10 mM coumarin in 2% sucrose stimuli, was given, and positive PER was calculated. The test was performed for 10 flies at a time, and the positive PER for each fly was calculated as 10% proboscis extension. The test was repeated four times for each fly strain, i.e. mutant, control, and rescue fly strains.
We performed tip recordings as previously described (Moon et al., 2006). We immobilized freshly enclosed flies by keeping them on ice and then inserted reference glass electrodes filled with Ringer’s solution into the thorax of the flies, extending the electrode towards their proboscis. We stimulated the sensilla with tastants dissolved in buffer solution in recording pipettes (10?20 μm tip diameter). We used 1 mM KCl or 30 mM tricholine citrate as the electrolyte for recording. The recording electrode was connected to a preamplifier (TastePROBE, Syntech, Hilversum, The Netherlands), and the signals were collected and amplified 10x, using a signal connection interface box (Syntech) in conjunction with a 100?3000 Hz band-pass filter. Recordings of action potentials were acquired using a 12-kHz sampling rate, and analyzed using Autospike 3.1 software (Syntech). First, we performed recordings on S6 sensilla with different concentrations of coumarin i.e. 0 mM, 0.1 mM and 1 mM for control flies. Finally, we performed recordings with the mutant and rescue flies from S5, S6 and S9 sensilla with 1 mM coumarin.
We developed our own protocol for egg laying (Poudel et al., 2015). Fifteen male and 15 female flies, all 2?3 days-old, were kept in a fresh food source and incubated at 25°C for 2 days. The experiment was divided into two parts: 6 h adaptation (starvation), and 18 h egg laying period, which completes 24 h circadian rhythm. For starvation, we kept the flies on 1% agarose on the egg laying apparatus, which was a 5 cm diameter petri dish divided by 4 mm spacer. We started the experiment exactly at 12 PM for starvation, and then at 6 PM we transferred the flies to the petri dish containing 1% agarose, with either 1 mM sucrose, or 5 mM sucrose plus indicated concentrations for coumarin. The flies were then allowed to lay eggs for 18 h. The flies were kept in a dark and humidified chamber for both starvation and oviposition. The numbers of eggs laid over 18 h were counted. The oviposition index was calculated, as previously described (Yang et al., 2008).
We performed survival assays with the control flies. We prepared three different combinations of food sources: one with 1% sucrose, and the other two with 1% sucrose plus 1 mM and 10 mM coumarin. We placed 10 male and 10 female flies, 3?4 days-old, on each of these food sources. The flies were observed every 12 h, and then transferred to new vials containing the same food source. The assay was performed for 72 h, by which time all the flies feeding on coumarin containing media were dead. The test was repeated 10 times.
All error bars represent SEMs. Single factor ANOVA with Scheffe’s analysis was used as a
Coumarin, a fragrant organic chemical compound, is considered a phenylpropanoid (Fig. 1A). It is produced by plants as a defense mechanism to repel predators. Coumarin is moderately toxic to the liver and kidneys in mammals (Lake, 1999); therefore, we decided to test its toxicity on insect models. The toxicity of coumarin was investigated by performing survival assay with control flies. Flies maintained in 1% sucrose lived for more than 72 h (Fig. 1B). However, 1 mM coumarin was sufficient to kill 50% of the flies in 45 h (LT50). This was reduced to 30 h (LT50) for 10 mM coumarin (Fig. 1B). All coumarin-feeding flies were dead before 72 h. This indicates that coumarin is also toxic to insects.
Coumarin is known to have a pleasant smell to humans. However, it also has a somewhat bitter-tasting anti-feedant effect. To determine whether the repellent behavior is actually mediated by its bitterness or by its odor, we expressed a proapoptosis gene (
To find the
The fly has multiple contact-chemosensory organs including a proboscis, legs, wings and genitalia. The main contact-chemosensory organs are the proboscis and legs. To further verify the roles of GRs in the labellum, we performed the proboscis extension response (PER) assay for coumarin with defective
Next, we performed tip recordings to elicit coumarin-induced action potentials from S5, S6 and S9 sensilla, which are known to be highly activated by coumarin (Weiss et al., 2011). Consistent with the behavioral defects of
These data support the hypothesis that the activity of GRs in the labellum is necessary to discriminate between nutritious food sources and toxic foods.
Recent study provides the evidence that three bitter-sensing GRs are enough to recapitulate L-canavanine receptor (GR33a, GR66a, and GR98b) in sweet neurons (Shim et al., 2015). To recapitulate a coumarin receptor in sweet neurons ectopically, we generated the flies expressing all the three
The site selection for the deposition of the egg in fruit fly is crucial for the safety of the developing larvae. The studies done till date support the idea that flies sense media using smell and taste for the oviposition in Drosophila (Dweck et al., 2013; Joseph and Herberlein, 2012; Yang et al., 2008).
Coumarin is an ovicidal compound (Nakajima and Kawazu, 1980). So our assumption was that female flies should avoid a coumarin-containing substrate for egg laying. We carried out egg laying assay with the same condition as binary food choice assay. 0.3 mM coumarin was not enough for the female flies to avoid egg laying as binary food choice assay (Figs. 1C, 4A, and 4B). Interestingly, only
The olfactory receptor is composed of Orco and specific OR, which resides in specific ORNs. However, GRs are distributed on overlapped sensilla. For example,
Haein Kim, Yong Taek Jeong, Min Sung Choi, Jaekyun Choi, Seok Jun Moon, and Jae Young Kwon
Mol. Cells 2017; 40(5): 331-338 https://doi.org/10.14348/molcells.2017.0028